Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control

Lee A. Baugh, Erica Hoe, J. Randall Flanagan

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

7 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

Certain hand-held tools alter the mapping between hand motion and motion of the tool end point that must be controlled in order to perform a task. For example, when using a pool cue, the motion of the cue tip is reversed relative to the hand. Previous studies have shown that the time required to initiate a reaching movement (Fernandez-Ruiz J, Wong W, Armstrong IT, Flanagan JR. Behav Brain Res 219: 8 -14, 2011), or correct an ongoing reaching movement (Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF. J Neurophysiol 104: 3084 -3104, 2010), is prolonged when the mapping between hand motion and motion of a cursor controlled by the hand is reversed. Here we show that these time costs can be significantly reduced when the reversal is instantiated by a virtual hand-held tool. Participants grasped the near end of a virtual tool, consisting of a rod connecting two circles, and moved the end point to displayed targets. In the reversal condition, the rod translated through, and rotated about, a pivot point such that there was a left-right reversal between hand and end point motion. In the nonreversal control, the tool translated with the hand. As expected, when only the two circles were presented, movement initiation and correction times were much longer in the reversal condition. However, when full vision of the tool was provided, the reaction time cost was almost eliminated. These results indicate that tools with complex kinematics can be efficiently incorporated into sensorimotor control mechanisms used in movement planning and online control.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)1954-1964
Number of pages11
JournalJournal of Neurophysiology
Volume108
Issue number7
DOIs
StatePublished - Oct 1 2012

Fingerprint

Biomechanical Phenomena
Hand
Cues
Costs and Cost Analysis
Reaction Time
Brain

Keywords

  • Movement correction
  • Movement planning
  • Sensorimotor control
  • Tool use
  • Visuomotor reversal

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Neuroscience(all)
  • Physiology

Cite this

Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control. / Baugh, Lee A.; Hoe, Erica; Randall Flanagan, J.

In: Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 108, No. 7, 01.10.2012, p. 1954-1964.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

@article{20f76e64d25b4bfabc11cb936af6e866,
title = "Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control",
abstract = "Certain hand-held tools alter the mapping between hand motion and motion of the tool end point that must be controlled in order to perform a task. For example, when using a pool cue, the motion of the cue tip is reversed relative to the hand. Previous studies have shown that the time required to initiate a reaching movement (Fernandez-Ruiz J, Wong W, Armstrong IT, Flanagan JR. Behav Brain Res 219: 8 -14, 2011), or correct an ongoing reaching movement (Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF. J Neurophysiol 104: 3084 -3104, 2010), is prolonged when the mapping between hand motion and motion of a cursor controlled by the hand is reversed. Here we show that these time costs can be significantly reduced when the reversal is instantiated by a virtual hand-held tool. Participants grasped the near end of a virtual tool, consisting of a rod connecting two circles, and moved the end point to displayed targets. In the reversal condition, the rod translated through, and rotated about, a pivot point such that there was a left-right reversal between hand and end point motion. In the nonreversal control, the tool translated with the hand. As expected, when only the two circles were presented, movement initiation and correction times were much longer in the reversal condition. However, when full vision of the tool was provided, the reaction time cost was almost eliminated. These results indicate that tools with complex kinematics can be efficiently incorporated into sensorimotor control mechanisms used in movement planning and online control.",
keywords = "Movement correction, Movement planning, Sensorimotor control, Tool use, Visuomotor reversal",
author = "Baugh, {Lee A.} and Erica Hoe and {Randall Flanagan}, J.",
year = "2012",
month = "10",
day = "1",
doi = "10.1152/jn.00157.2012",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "108",
pages = "1954--1964",
journal = "Journal of Neurophysiology",
issn = "0022-3077",
publisher = "American Physiological Society",
number = "7",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - Hand-held tools with complex kinematics are efficiently incorporated into movement planning and online control

AU - Baugh, Lee A.

AU - Hoe, Erica

AU - Randall Flanagan, J.

PY - 2012/10/1

Y1 - 2012/10/1

N2 - Certain hand-held tools alter the mapping between hand motion and motion of the tool end point that must be controlled in order to perform a task. For example, when using a pool cue, the motion of the cue tip is reversed relative to the hand. Previous studies have shown that the time required to initiate a reaching movement (Fernandez-Ruiz J, Wong W, Armstrong IT, Flanagan JR. Behav Brain Res 219: 8 -14, 2011), or correct an ongoing reaching movement (Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF. J Neurophysiol 104: 3084 -3104, 2010), is prolonged when the mapping between hand motion and motion of a cursor controlled by the hand is reversed. Here we show that these time costs can be significantly reduced when the reversal is instantiated by a virtual hand-held tool. Participants grasped the near end of a virtual tool, consisting of a rod connecting two circles, and moved the end point to displayed targets. In the reversal condition, the rod translated through, and rotated about, a pivot point such that there was a left-right reversal between hand and end point motion. In the nonreversal control, the tool translated with the hand. As expected, when only the two circles were presented, movement initiation and correction times were much longer in the reversal condition. However, when full vision of the tool was provided, the reaction time cost was almost eliminated. These results indicate that tools with complex kinematics can be efficiently incorporated into sensorimotor control mechanisms used in movement planning and online control.

AB - Certain hand-held tools alter the mapping between hand motion and motion of the tool end point that must be controlled in order to perform a task. For example, when using a pool cue, the motion of the cue tip is reversed relative to the hand. Previous studies have shown that the time required to initiate a reaching movement (Fernandez-Ruiz J, Wong W, Armstrong IT, Flanagan JR. Behav Brain Res 219: 8 -14, 2011), or correct an ongoing reaching movement (Gritsenko V, Kalaska JF. J Neurophysiol 104: 3084 -3104, 2010), is prolonged when the mapping between hand motion and motion of a cursor controlled by the hand is reversed. Here we show that these time costs can be significantly reduced when the reversal is instantiated by a virtual hand-held tool. Participants grasped the near end of a virtual tool, consisting of a rod connecting two circles, and moved the end point to displayed targets. In the reversal condition, the rod translated through, and rotated about, a pivot point such that there was a left-right reversal between hand and end point motion. In the nonreversal control, the tool translated with the hand. As expected, when only the two circles were presented, movement initiation and correction times were much longer in the reversal condition. However, when full vision of the tool was provided, the reaction time cost was almost eliminated. These results indicate that tools with complex kinematics can be efficiently incorporated into sensorimotor control mechanisms used in movement planning and online control.

KW - Movement correction

KW - Movement planning

KW - Sensorimotor control

KW - Tool use

KW - Visuomotor reversal

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84867182042&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=84867182042&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1152/jn.00157.2012

DO - 10.1152/jn.00157.2012

M3 - Article

C2 - 22773780

AN - SCOPUS:84867182042

VL - 108

SP - 1954

EP - 1964

JO - Journal of Neurophysiology

JF - Journal of Neurophysiology

SN - 0022-3077

IS - 7

ER -